Skip to main content

Pragmatic skills in people with Williams syndrome: the perception of families

Abstract

Background

One of the most challenging linguistic areas in people with Williams Syndrome throughout their evolutionary stage is the development of pragmatic skills. The research conducted so far highlights specific problems concerning adaptation to the linguistic context and interlocutors, language comprehension, as well as other aspects interfering with verbal communication. However, until now, most scientific evidence has been based on personal assessments of this group. In a complementary manner, the goal of this study was to discover the level of pragmatic skills of people with Williams Syndrome from the point of view of the families. The sample consisted of 34 families belonging to the Williams Syndrome Association of Spain. The assessment instrument was the pragmatic awareness questionnaire, which includes 26 items related to different aspects that are part of the pragmatic area on a Likert-type scale.

Results

The results indicate that, families consider there to be a regular to low level in all the areas assessed. The strong points seem to lie in the paralinguistic aspects, while the weakest factors are those related to the understanding of figurative language.

Conclusions

Therefore, it is necessary to continue insisting on the importance of language intervention in this group throughout its development to improve its level of linguistic competence.

Introduction

Williams Syndrome (hereinafter ‘WS’) is a genetic disorder caused by a deletion in chromosome 7q11.23 [76]. WS is estimated to affect 1 in 20,000/50,000 live births worldwide [49].

The first studies conducted with this population began to show an atypical neuropsychological profile characterized by peaks and valleys in the various abilities [25]. For example, Bellugi et al. [7] showed that abilities such as language was well preserved in contrast to other abilities such as visuospatial construction cognition, attention, or visuospatial memory [40]. Subsequent works have confirmed that, although it is true that people with WS exhibit a series of characteristic features, there is a profile that is incredibly open to individual variability [43].

Although the clinical profile of this population has led some authors to interpret WS as an argument evidencing the feasibility of the theory of modular language functioning [60], there is currently much research suggesting that the dissociation between linguistic production and cognitive functioning does not exist in people with WS. For this reason, when analyzing these abilities in-depth, specific difficulties of people with WS become apparent in the different linguistic areas [53]. For example, at the phonetic-phonological level, there is unintelligible articulation, phonological paraphasia and a general delay in acquisition manifested through various phonological simplification processes—such as assimilation or omission, malapropism, and epenthesis—persisting beyond the expected norm [25]. However, it is also interesting to note that both phonological segmentation tasks and word and pseudoword repetition tasks can lead to scores that are very similar to those of subjects with typical development [20, 37, 57, 74].

Concerning the morpho-syntactic level, people with WS again seem to manifest profiles below those corresponding to typical development [24]. For example, difficulties in the correct use of the morphemes of time, gender and number are particularly noteworthy,and even the understanding, production, and repetition of structurally complex utterances [24, 55, 58].

Regarding the lexical-semantic level, it has been observed that there are several differences in this area in people with WS. Although some strong points are lexical decision tasks and semantic association, they experience many more problems in other tasks such as naming pictures, manifesting specific difficulties in word retrieval, possibly because of lesser sustained attention [24]. For their part, Mervis and Becerra [39] reported a relative preservation of the lexical component in people with WS and Garayzábal and Cuetos [20] evidenced the existence of significant differences between the relational and conceptual lexicon. Along this line, the study conducted by Moraleda and López [43] revealed how, although people with WS obtain scores below what one might expect based on their mental age, the passive lexicon of this population increases in proportion to their chronological age.

In the realm of pragmatics and language usage, there exists notable divergence among various research studies. While less recent research assures us that people with WS do not experience problems in this area [44, 50, 75], other studies show that this area is delayed when compared, for example, to the development of the semantic area [28], Van den [69].

Specifically with regard to the pragmatic characteristics of people with WS, there has been evidence of poor turn-taking skills [4, 69], difficulties with understanding figurative language [71], excessive use of emotional language [70] and limited use of textual markers in narrative construction [13], among other aspects. More recent studies indicate that in many cases, WS is accompanied by a Social Communication disorder [14], following previous studies that associated WS with social communication deficits, in the form of atypical social interaction (conversational exchange structure, turn-taking and information transfer) and conversational insufficiency [59].

For their part, authors such as Gallardo [18], Lacroix et al. [33] and Royston et al. [54] have shown the existence of a specific deficit in the Gricean principle of cooperation [23] manifesting itself through a series of alterations in conversational maxims. Specifically, it has been observed that, although the linking of conversational turns occurs smoothly [32], constant overlapping [16] makes it difficult to understand the relevance of the interlocutors’ contributions (maxim of relation). This, in turn, makes it difficult to take adequate reactive turns [72]. In this sense, Aroca and Liras [10] and Diez-Itza et al. [13] also attribute the continuous changes in discursive themes to difficulties with the maxim of relation, which is crucial to determining when a contribution is relevant to the communicative situation. Likewise, various research studies have found that some of the lexical-semantic peculiarities of WS, such as the choice of atypical and infrequent words [38], can be explained by difficulties with the maxim of manner, which is necessary for adapting language to the communicative context [54, 66]. In a similar vein, certain authors associate the characteristic language patterns of individuals with Williams syndrome (WS) with challenges in managing information overload. This implies that individuals with WS may struggle to discern instances of excessive speech or situations where insufficient information is conveyed. [3].

There are also many research studies addressing the study of the theory of mind in people with WS [6, 30, 54, 56, 73].

Thus, some authors suggest that people with WS experience a dissociation between their social and cognitive profiles in terms of mentalizing skills (understood as the ability to make inferences about the thoughts and beliefs of other people), although the evidence is contradictory. Initial research suggested that these abilities were preserved in WS, unlike other cognitive abilities [31]. However, Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan [64] demonstrated that the socio-perceptual components of the theory of mind, such as facial expression, body language or vocal prosody would be preserved while the socio-cognitive components such as language and understanding of false beliefs [63] would be impaired [45]. However, Porter et al. [47] showed that theory of mind skills in people with WS are below what would be expected for their mental age when they are assessed using a non-verbal task, not being able to depend on their verbal skills. Therefore, it seems difficult to reach a clear conclusion about mentalizing abilities in people with WS, although their potential difficulties in this domain are more closely related to the social-cognitive component than to the social-perceptive one [67].

All this information suggests the existence of a specific difficulty in the assessed pragmatic skills of people with WS. However, there is little scientific literature on the perception of parents concerning these specific difficulties. Bearing in mind that it is families that spend the most time with them in natural contexts and given the importance of being direct observers [42], the goal of this study was to determine the pragmatic characteristics of people with WS from the point of view of their families.

Method

Participants

The sample comprises a total of 34 families with children diagnosed with WS, aged between 8 and 20 years, 52.9% female and 47.1% male, residing in Spain. Specifically, 26 fathers between the ages of 32 and 65 and 18 mothers between the ages of 29 and 59 participated in this study.

It is noteworthy that all people with WS presented a certificate of disability equal to or greater than 33%. A degree of disability of 33% indicates a disability that prevents you from conducting certain activities but does not prevent you from functioning in your current environment. It is regulated by the Spanish public administration and is issued by the Assessment and Orientation Teams made up of at least a doctor, psychologist, and social worker. About people with WS, it should be noted that 50% were attended by a speech and language therapist. The participants belong to the Williams Syndrome Association of Spain (ASWE). They all speak Spanish as their first language.

Instruments

To conduct this study, the pragmatic awareness questionnaire (PAQ) [52] was distributed to the families of people with WS. This questionnaire follows the integrative line of the ‘PerLa’ corpus [17] of clinical data on aphasia and evaluation protocols such as the Rapid Pragmatic Evaluation Protocol—Revised (PREP-R) [15] and draws inspiration from the Pragmatic Protocol Manual by Prutting and Kirchner [48], from which the categories that most closely resemble the WS profile in the PAQ have been selected and introduced (Additional file 1). This questionnaire had already been used previously in other studies in the population with intellectual disabilities [9, 52].

The PAQ is made up of blocks, divided into 26 items that are distributed as ordinal variables, following a 5-point Likert-type scale, where 1 = very bad, 2 = bad, 3 = fair, 4 = good and 5 = very good. The wording of some items has been simplified and examples have been added for ease of understanding.

Each block of the questionnaire pertains to a different area. In block I (items 1–2), intelligibility (effectiveness in understanding spoken voice) and paralanguage (intonation, volume, etc.) are assessed. In block II (items 3–8), aspects of non-verbal communication are measured, such as the physical contact maintained during the communicative act or the distance established between the interlocutors, along with factors such as body posture, gaze, facial expression, and the use of gestures. Block III (items 9–10) focuses on lexical competence and cohesion. Block IV (items 11–13) assesses semantic-pragmatic capacities, related to the understanding of irony and humor, and the interpretation of ambiguous statements. Block V (items 14–16) refers to the morpho-syntactic characteristics of the discourse. In block VI (item 17), the ability to adapt to the interlocutor and the communicative situation is assessed. Lastly, block VII (items 18–25) focuses on the amount and relevance of information in communicative exchanges, response time, control over turn-taking, the degree of acceptance and coherence of the topics put forward by the subject and the ability to follow the conversation. The last item (26) is an open qualitative question concerning general communication, where the perception and concerns that families of people with WS may have about their level of pragmatic skills are collected in writing.

The criteria to measure reliability were: (a) Cronbach’s alpha, which allowed obtaining a value of 0.972; (b) the standardized alpha (0.969), and (c) the correlation of each item with the total of the scale (item-total correlation), which for all the items exceed the value 0.20.

Procedure

First, the Williams Syndrome Association of Spain—the center from which the participants were selected—was contacted. A document was drawn up with a brief description of the study, its goals, the pertinent instructions, and the importance of family participation. Likewise, the document contains a Google Forms link to the questionnaire that was used, where the informed consent also appears. After approval by the Association and acceptance by the Ethics Committee of the University’s Faculty of Health Sciences, the families were given the document containing the questionnaire to be filled out. The approximate time needed to answer all the questions was 15 min.

Once all the questionnaires were completed, the data collected was subjected to a descriptive statistical analysis for the purposes of studying parents’ perception of their children’s level of pragmatic skills and the characteristics thereof. For this research, statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 24.0. The most important descriptive statistics were obtained. To analyze the correlation between variables, Spearman’s Rho test was used.

Results

The results obtained have shown that, from the parents’ perspective, people with WS experience difficulties in the various items of pragmatics. The general averages of the results can be seen in Table 1.

Table 1 Mean scores in the sections of the pragmatic awareness questionnaire

The medium score of each block was calculate and the results were: block I (intelligibility and paralanguage) = 2.99 (0.12), block II (non-verbal communication) = 3.43 (0.65), block III (lexical competence and cohesion) = 2.96 (0.25), block IV (semantic-pragmatic capacities) = 2.01 (0.37), block V (morpho-syntactic characteristics) = 2.56 (0.18), block VI (ability to adapt to the interlocutor and to the communicative situation) = 2.55 (0.48) and block VII (relevance of information) = 2.67 (0.41). The results indicated that significant differences appeared between block IV and blocks I, II and III (p < 0.05) and block II and the blocks IV, V and VI (p < 0.05).

Regarding the development of the responses offered by family members, it has been observed that items that in the perception about intelligibility of speech, 16% of family members consider that intelligibility is awful in people with WS.

In the item that evaluates suprasegmental aspects such as intonation, rhythm, etc., the results about the perception of families are: 32% positive (excellent or good), 53% regular and 15% negative (bad or very bad). As can be seen, the only item showing a score above 4 (good) is the one referring to facial expression. Most of the items related to non-verbal language (3–8) are between 3 and 4 points. The items that collect the development and the various subareas of oral language (1–2, 9–25) have average scores between 2 and 3 points, which is manifested in the parents’ perception that these aspects are quite abnormal in people with WS (between bad and fair). It should be noted that there were especially low scores in the areas of understanding figurative language with a score of between bad and very bad, interpreting ambiguous expressions and comments (1.86) and understanding and reacting to irony (1.69).

For example, analyzing the item that talks about the proximity that is maintained with the interlocutor in a conversation, the parents have referred very well or well in 38% in WS but we also found scores that refer very poorly or poorly with 21% in WS. Regarding the item that speaks of the use of physical contact in communication situations, better results are concluded in WS with 68% of responses that refer very well or well compared to 12% that consider very badly or badly. Continuing the analysis with the next items, we find the body posture they maintain in a conversation. Better scores are observed in WS with 53% of relatives reporting scores of very good or good. The percentage is similar in the body movements of the arms and hands (56%). The best score was found in facial expression (88%).

As for the items referring to expression, the number of words they know and use in their language in WS is 44% positive (very good or good), 41% regular and 15% negative (bad or very bad). The results don´t improve in word construction (35% positive, 29% regular and 36% negative) and construction of phrases and sentences (24% positive, 44% regular and 32% negative).

To finish, we analyze the last item of this block which includes the interpretation of ambiguous expressions and comments, such as set phrases or metaphors. The results are considered very well or well in 3%. The percentage of answers referring to bad or very bad is 56%. We continue with another block that values understanding and reactions to irony and humor. The irony is considered 3% positive, 18% regular and 79% negative and in the case of humor, the results are slightly better (30% positive, 29% regular and 41% negative).

With regard to the last item (26), which is formulated by means of an open question about parents’ general feelings about the communication of their children with WS and their concern in this regard, the majority of the parents were quite concerned about aspects such as the development of verbal communication, the adaptation of linguistic expression to the context, the understanding of language, turn-taking and specific difficulties when engaging in conversations with their peers and expressing their feelings. All these aspects occur continuously and have a negative impact on communication, leading to great concern amongst most families.

Discussion

The results obtained in this research study have shown that people with WS experience pragmatic difficulties throughout their development from the point of view of their parents, especially concerning those characteristics relating to the use of oral language in communicative contexts. This research confirms that subjects with WS have difficulty following pragmatic rules [34, 46]. Furthermore, these data follow the line of Agüero and Garayzábal [2] in that the pragmatic characteristics related to verbal aspects, such as the relevance and coherence of the discourse, are abnormal. However, the results do not coincide with the above study regarding non-verbal aspects because relative strengths were found in questions related to block I and block II related to intelligibility, paralanguage, and non-verbal communication, such as eye contact, prosody, or voice control, among others. In our case, parents considered these paralinguistic aspects to be the most developed areas in their children with WS. Nor do our results coincide with others suggesting that people with WS make inappropriate use of prosody and pepper their narrations with excessive onomatopoeia and noises [8, 50, 75], even beyond adolescence [35].

Some studies show that in relation to the conversational competence of people with WS, more than 25% of the expressions were inappropriate, which in half of the cases was due to problems of syntax and expressive semantics and an insufficient amount of information provided [61]. Along these lines, the results of our research show that lexical competence does seem to be more preserved (block III), while syntactic competence (block V) seems to show more difficulty compared to the rest of the blocks evaluated.

Regarding discourse comprehension, the results show that the comprehension of figurative and metaphorical language (understanding irony, jokes, metaphors or double meanings) is very poor [19], so the responses belonging to block IV about semantic-pragmatic capacities (understanding of irony and humor, and the interpretation of ambiguous statements) are those that obtain the lowest scores, assuming a weakness for people with Williams Syndrome. It seems clear that people with WS may have more difficulty understanding non-literal language in areas such as sarcasm, metaphor, and simile when compared to people with typical development of the same chronological age [22]. These same authors point out that these differences disappear between people with WS, and people matched for mental age. These results suggest that these difficulties could be related to intellectual disability and, in this case, indicate that the linguistic and cognitive systems on which the understanding of non-literal language is based interact and integrate differently in individuals with WS compared to those with typical development. Furthermore, according to the authors, the difficulties in understanding sarcasm observed in people with WS could be because sarcasm demands more executive functions, such as cognitive flexibility and context integration.

Concerning the ability to adapt to the interlocutor and the communicative situation, the families assign a low to regular score. It seems, therefore, that people with WS also have difficulties in narratives; when explaining the plot, they introduce irrelevant personal experiences and these tend to lack coherence and cohesion [34, 36, 51, 55, 62]. The parents’ perception also coincides with research confirming that difficulties in turn-taking and following the topic of conversation are distinctive features in this group [16, 41, 54, 65, 74].

In addition, we must be aware that these characteristics do not occur in isolation, as difficulties in understanding the opinions of others [29, 64]—and even in maintaining and establishing linguistic relationships with other interlocutors—lead to personal withdrawal. As a result, people with WS tend not to want to participate in social interactions with their peers [11, 63, 68]. In addition, these characteristics of pragmatic competence in people with WS seem to be very similar to those presented by people with autism spectrum disorder [9, 52]. Hence, individuals with Williams Syndrome appear to exhibit limited linguistic variability within their areas of expertise. Moreover, their conversations tend to revolve around subjects they are familiar with and proficient in, often influencing challenges in social interactions.

Regarding the limitations of the study, it is interesting to highlight, on the one hand, that the individual variability of people with WS within the syndrome must be taken into account, and on the other hand, that the ages of people with WS are so diverse and therefore their language development may be dependent on their age. On the other hand, although there are more and more studies that include language development questionnaires completed by parents in different population [1, 5, 26, 27], it would be interesting to complement our data with those provided by professionals working with people with WS on a daily basis.

In conclusion, if we wish to improve the communication skills of people with WS, we should continue insisting that linguistic work and intervention take place throughout all stages of their development, given the pragmatic language difficulties they experience. The pragmatic profile in people with Williams Syndrome continues to present alterations even in adulthood [14]. So, pragmatic intervention can be effective in SW does take advantage of the elements related to the retention and verbalization of the events of the narrative and thus lay the foundation for the organization and understanding of the discourse [12]. However and to complement this work, following García-Medall and Arranz-López [21] and Sotillo et al. [58], future research efforts on the relationship between the mental and linguistic skills of people with WS should continue to be undertaken, with a view to improving their pragmatic competence and, as a result, their relationship with other interlocutors.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the research team, but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under licence for the current study and so are not publicly available. The data are, however, available from the authors upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ASWE:

Williams syndrome association of Spain

PAQ:

Pragmatic awareness questionnaire

WS:

Williams syndrome

References

  1. Abraham MDV, Brenca RM. Análisis psicométrico de la evaluación del aspecto pragmático del lenguaje infantil: Batería ICRA-A. Interdisciplinaria. 2014;31(1):139–61.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Agüero J, Garayzábal E. Características Morfosintácticas de Cuatro Afectados por el Síndrome de Williams. Revista Síndrome de Williams, 1998;1.

  3. Alonso MIG, Gómez MBM. Comportamiento, lenguaje y cognición de algunos síndromes que cursan con discapacidad intelectual. Revista INFAD de Psicología Int J Develop Educ Psychol. 2017;4(1):55–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Asada K, Tomiwa K, Okada M, Itakura S. Fluent language with impaired pragmatics in children with Williams syndrome. J Neurolinguist. 2010;23(6):540–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Auza A, Peñaloza Castillo C. Factores individuales y familiares en el Trastorno del Desarrollo del Lenguaje (TDL). Iztapalapa Revista de ciencias sociales y humanidades. 2019;40(86):41–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Barak B, Feng G. Neurobiology of social behavior abnormalities in autism and Williams syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(5):647–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Bellugi U, Bihrle A, Jernigan T, Trauner D, Doherty S. Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1990;37(S6):115–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bellugi U, Wang PP, Jernigan TL. Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. Atypic Cognit Deficits Develop Disord Implicat Brain Func. 1994;23:23–56.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Camargo L, y Cortés R. Desarrollo y mejora de la competencia pragmática en niños y adolescentes con trastorno del espectro autista y lenguaje funcional. Normas. 2020. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7203/Normas.v10i1.19279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Candela de Aroca MT, Liras Pisano N. Las alteraciones en la teoría de la mente y de la coherencia central justifican las dificultades en el área de la pragmática en personas con SW? (Bachelor’s thesis). 2015.

  11. Davies M, Howlin P, Udwin O. Adults with Williams syndrome: Preliminary study of social, emotional, and behavioural difficulties. Br J Psychiatry. 1998;172(3):273–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Díez Itza E, MartínezLópez V, Miranda Fernández M, Antón Pérez MA. El recuerdo de la estructura narrativa en el síndrome de Down y en el síndrome de Williams: investigación para la intervención en el nivel pragmático. Revista Infad. 2006;1(3):353–66.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Diez-Itza E, Martínez V, Pérez V, Fernández-Urquiza M. Explicit oral narrative intervention for students with Williams syndrome. Front Psychol. 2018;8:2337.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Diez-Itza E, Viejo A, Fernández-Urquiza M. Pragmatic profiles of adults with Fragile X syndrome and Williams syndrome. Brain Sci. 2022;12(3):385.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Fernández-Urquiza M, Díaz Martínez F, Moreno Campos V, Lázaro López-Villaseñor M, Simón López T. PREP-R. Protocolo Rápido de Evaluación Pragmática Revisado. Valencia: Universidad de Valencia. Search in. 2015.

  16. Fisher MH, Morin L. Addressing social skills deficits in adults with Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2017;71:77–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gallardo-Paúls B, Sanmartín Sáez J. Afasia fluente. Materiales para su estudio. (Volumen 01 del Corpus PerLA). Universitat de Valencia. 2005.

  18. Gallardo B. Habilidades lingüísticas pragmáticas en el Síndrome de Williams. Rev Invest. 2010;70:71–93.

    Google Scholar 

  19. GarayzabalHeinze E, Prieto MF, Sampaio A, Gonçalves Ó. Cross-linguistic assessment of verbal production from a narrative task in Williams syndrome. Psicothema. 2007;19(3):428–34.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Garayzábal Heinze E, Cuetos Vega F. Aprendizaje de la lectura en los niños con síndrome de Williams. Psicothema, 2008;20(4).

  21. García-Medall J, López EA. Perfil psicolingüístico y trastornos de comportamiento en un adulto con síndrome de Williams. Moenia, 2015.

  22. Godbee K, Porter M. Comprehension of sarcasm, metaphor, and simile in Williams syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2013;48(6):651–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Grice HP. Logic and conversation. In: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). Brill. 1975.

  24. Heinze EG, Vega FC. Lexico-semantic processing in Williams syndrome. Psicothema. 2010;22(4):732–8.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hidalgo IG, Garayzábal E. Diferencias fonológicas entre síndromes del neurodesarrollo: evidencias a partir de los procesos de simplificación fonológica más frecuentes. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia. 2019;9(2):81–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ibáñez-Rodríguez A, Ahufinger N, Ferinu L, García-Arch J, Andreu L, Sanz-Torrent M. Dificultades sociales, emocionales y victimización específica por el lenguaje en el trastorno del desarrollo del lenguaje. Revista de Logopedia, foniatría y audiología. 2021;41(1):40–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Jiménez JE, Rodríguez C. Experiencia con el lenguaje impreso e indicadores socioculturales asociados a los diferentes subtipos disléxicos. Psicothema. 2008;20(3):341–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. John AE, Dobson LA, Thomas L, Mervis CB. Pragmatic abilities of children with Williams syndrome: a longitudinal examination. Front Psychol. 2012;3:199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. John AE, Rowe ML, Mervis CB. Referential communication skills of children with Williams syndrome: understanding when messages are not adequate. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2009;114(2):85–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Kampis D, Fogd D, Kovács ÁM. Nonverbal components of theory of Mind in typical and atypical development. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;48:54–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Karmiloff-Smith A, Klima E, Bellugi U, Grant J, Baron-Cohen S. Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. J Cogn Neurosci. 1995;7(2):196–208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Klein-Tasman BP, van der Fluit F, Mervis CB. Autism spectrum symptomatology in children with Williams syndrome who have phrase speech or fluent language. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(9):3037–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Lacroix A, Famelart N, Guidetti M. Language and emotional abilities in children with Williams syndrome and children with autism spectrum disorder: similarities and differences. Pediatric Health Med Therapeut. 2016;7:89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Laws G, Bishop DV. Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2004;39(1):45–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Loveall SJ, Hawthorne K, Gaines M. A meta-analysis of prosody in autism, Williams syndrome, and Down syndrome. J Commun Disord. 2021;89: 106055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Maratsos M, Matheny L. Language specificity and elasticity: brain and clinical syndrome studies. Annu Rev Psychol. 1994;45(1):487–516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Martínez López V, Antón Pérez MA, Miranda M, Díez Itza E. Critical indexes of development persistence of phonological processes in a Williams syndrome linguistic corpus. In: La lingüística ante el reto de la aplicación clínica: actas del II Congreso Internacional de Lingüística Clínica. Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia.

  38. Meeuwsen, S. (2017). Lexical Abilities in Williams Syndrome (master’s thesis).

  39. Mervis CB, Becerra AM. Language and communicative development in Williams syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(1):3–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Mervis CB, John AE. Vocabulary abilities of children with Williams syndrome: Strengths, weaknesses, and relation to visuospatial construction ability. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(4):967–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Meyerson MD, Frank RA. Language, speech, and hearing in Williams syndrome: intervention approaches and research needs. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29(2):258–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. MoraledaSepúlveda E, LópezResa P. Evaluating quality of life in families with Williams syndrome patients. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2021;19(1):1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Moraleda E, López P. Analysis of receptive vocabulary development in Williams syndrome. Open J Modern Linguist. 2020;10(06):804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Murphy MB, Greenberg F, Wilson G, Hughes M, DiLiberti J. Williams syndrome in twins. Am J Med Genet. 1990;37(S6):97–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Niego A, Benítez-Burraco A. Autism and Williams syndrome: truly mirror conditions in the socio-cognitive domain? Int J Develop Disabil, 2020; 1–17.

  46. Philofsky A, Fidler DJ, Hepburn S. Pragmatic language profiles of school-age children with autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome. Am J Speech Lang Patohol. 2007;16(4):368–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Porter MA, Coltheart M, Langdon R. Theory of mind in Williams syndrome assessed using a nonverbal task. J Autism Develop Disord. 2008;38:806–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Prutting CA, Kittchner DM. A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. J Speech Hear Disord. 1987;52(2):105–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Puente Ferreras A, Fernández Lozano MP, Alvarado Izquierdo JM, Jiménez Rodríguez V. Williams syndrome: a rare disease with contradictory symptomatology. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología. 2011;43(2):217–28.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Reilly J, Klima ES, Bellugi U. Once more with feeling: affect and language in atypical populations. Dev Psychopathol. 1990;2(4):367–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Reilly J, Losh M, Bellugi U, Wulfeck B. “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain Lang. 2004;88(2):229–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Rodríguez F. La conciencia pragmática de adultos con síndrome de Asperger. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología. 2012;32:21–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Rossi NF, Souza DHD, Moretti-Ferreira D, Giacheti CM. Perfil da fluência da fala na síndrome de Williams-Beuren: estudo preliminar. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica. 2009;21:107–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Royston R, Waite J, Howlin P. Williams syndrome: recent advances in our understanding of cognitive, social, and psychological functioning. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2019;32(2):60–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Rubba J, Klima ES. Preposition use in a speaker with Williams syndrome: some cognitive grammar proposals. Center Res Lang Newslett. 1991;3:3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Serrano-Juárez CA, Prieto-Corona B, Rodríguez-Camacho M, Venegas-Vega CA, Yáñez-Téllez M, Silva-Pereyra J, De León Miranda MA. An exploration of social cognition in children with different degrees of genetic deletion in Williams syndrome. J Autism Develop Disord. 2021;51(5):1695–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Sotillo M, Garayzábal E. Socio-communicative abilities and linguistic abilities in Williams’ Syndrome. In: Poster Presentado en IXth European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Spetses (pp. 1–5) 1999.

  58. Sotillo M, García-Nogales MÁ, Campos R. Lenguaje y teoría de la mente: El caso del Síndrome de Williams. Infancia y aprendizaje. 2007;30(3):459–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Stojanovik V. Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. J Neurolinguist. 2006;19(2):157–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Stojanovik V, van Ewijk L. Do children with Williams syndrome have unusual vocabularies? J Neurolinguist. 2008;21(1):18–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Stojanovik V, Perkins M, Howard S. Language and conversational abilities in Williams syndrome: How good is good? Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2001;36(S1):234–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Stojanovik V, Perkins M, Howard S. Williams syndrome and specific language impairment do not support claims for developmental double dissociations and innate modularity. J Neurolinguist. 2004;17(6):403–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Sullivan K, Winner E, Tager-Flusberg H. Can adolescents with Williams syndrome tell the difference between lies and jokes? Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23(1–2):85–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Tager-Flusberg H, Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition. 2000;76(1):59–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Temple CM, Almazan M, Sherwood S. Lexical skills in Williams syndrome: a cognitive neuropsychological analysis. J Neurolinguist. 2002;15(6):463–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Thurman AJ, Fisher MH. The Williams syndrome social phenotype: Disentangling the contributions of social interest and social difficulties. In: International review of research in developmental disabilities (Vol. 49, pp. 191–227). Academic Press. 2015.

  67. Treichel N, Dukes D, Barisnikov K, Samson AC. How cognitive, social, and emotional profiles impact humor appreciation: sense of humor in autism spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome. Humor. 2022;35(1):113–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Udwin O, Yule W, Martin N. Cognitive abilities and behavioural characteristics of children with idiopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1987;28(2):297–309.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Van Den Heuvel E, Manders E, Swillen A, Zink I. Developmental trajectories of structural and pragmatic language skills in school-aged children with Williams syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2016;60(10):903–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Van Herwegen J, Aznar A, Tenenbaum H. The use of emotions in narratives in Williams syndrome. J Commun Disord. 2014;50:1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Van Herwegen J, Dimitriou D, Rundblad G. Development of novel metaphor and metonymy comprehension in typically developing children and Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(4):1300–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Vivanti G, Hamner T, Lee NR. Neurodevelopmental disorders affecting sociability: recent research advances and future directions in autism spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18(12):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Vivanti G, Hocking DR, Fanning P, Dissanayake C. The social nature of overimitation: insights from Autism and Williams syndrome. Cognition. 2017;161:10–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Volterra V, Capirci O, Pezzini G, Sabbadini L, Vicari S. Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Cortex. 1996;32(4):663–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Wang PP, Bellugi U. Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and cognitive neuroscience. Am J Dis Child. 1993;147(11):1246–51.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Williams J, Barrat-Boyes B, Lowe J. Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circulation. 1961;24:1311–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the Spanish Williams Syndrome Association and its families who contributed to and participated in this study.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, EMS and PLR; methodology, EMS and PLR; software, EMS and PLR; validation, EMS and PLR; formal analysis, EMS and PLR; investigation, EMS and PLR; writing—original draft preparation, EMS and PLR; writing—review and editing, EMS and PLR; supervision, EMS and PLR. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Esther Moraleda Sepúlveda.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Inte-grated Area of Talavera de la Reina (Project identification code 32/2021).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Items which are evaluated in the test PAQ.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sepúlveda, E.M., Resa, P.L. Pragmatic skills in people with Williams syndrome: the perception of families. Orphanet J Rare Dis 19, 95 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13023-024-03016-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13023-024-03016-0

Keywords