Skip to main content

Neurobehavioral and developmental profiles: genotype–phenotype correlations in individuals with Cornelia de Lange syndrome

Abstract

Background

Cornelia de Lange (CdLS) is a rare genetic disorder that affects most body systems. Variants in multiple genes including NIPBL and SMC1A, can cause the syndrome. To date, literature on genotype–phenotype associations in individuals with CdLS is extremely limited, although studies suggest some differences in clinical phenotype severity across variants. This study aimed to examine and compare neurobehavioral differences and developmental variability across CdLS genes, specifically NIPBL and SMC1A, and identify genotype–phenotype correlations.

Participants and methods

This patient-reported outcomes study included accessing data from the Coordination of Rare Diseases registry at Sanford. Parents of a total of 26 children/adults with CdLS and a known variant in NIPBL (Mean age = 20.46 years, SD = 11.21) and 12 with a known variant in SMC1A (Mean age = 11.08 years, SD = 9.04) completed a series of questionnaires regarding their child’s developmental history. This included attainment of common language and motor milestones, intervention history, and behavior functioning. Developmental history and reported behavior regulation difficulties were compared across variant groups.

Results

Overall, individuals with a pathogenic variant in NIPBL or SMC1A were similarly delayed across motor and language milestones with about 70% not using phrase speech and 30–50% not walking by 5 years of age. However, those with NIPBL variants showed more severity in behavioral phenotype, namely with more repetitive behaviors, tantrums, and withdrawn behaviors. In addition, these individuals were more likely than those with SMC1A variants to demonstrate self-injurious behaviors, and anxiety. Both groups yielded a similar proportion of participants who participated in speech and occupational therapy, however those with SMC1A variants were more likely to engage in physical therapy. Both clinical groups report low rate of communicative or assistive device use despite a large proportion of participants never mastering single word or sentence use.

Conclusions

Study results are consistent with recent investigations highlighting more severe behavioral phenotype, particularly autistic features, anxiety, and behavior regulation challenges, among those with NIPBL variants albeit comparable developmental milestones. Both groups endorsed very elevated attention problems. Findings highlight importance of early interventions, including behavioral health services.

Introduction

Cornelia de Lange syndrome (CdLS; MIM 122470) is a rare neurogenetic disorder caused by pathogenic variants in genes related to transcription regulation, particularly cohesin functions. The syndrome is typically characterized by limb abnormalities, growth and developmental delays, intellectual disability, unique dysmorphic facial features and multisystem impairment [15, 13]. Affected individuals with CdLS commonly have behavioral issues, presenting with autistic features, primarily repetitive or inflexible behaviors [4, 20], anxiety [17], attention problems, hyperactivity and self-injurious behaviors [4]. Notably, a recent consensus statement on the diagnosis and clinical management of CdLS has implicated both classic and atypical forms of CdLS, contending a spectrum of phenotypic features [14].

Molecular genetic investigations have revealed that multiple genetic variants can cause the classic or atypical CdLS phenotypes. A pathogenic variant in NIPBL is responsible for the majority of cases of CdLS, more commonly those with classic syndrome phenotype (~ 60–70%, [14]). Variants in SMC1A make up about 5% of those with CdLS [11], largely presenting with the non-classic form, although there is a group of individuals with loss of function variants in SMC1A presenting with a Rett syndrome-like phenotype [5]. Variants in RAD21, SMC3, BRD4, and ANKRD11 have also been seen among those with atypical CdLS [3, 7, 9, 10, 18], while pathogenic HDAC8 variants have been linked to both classic and non-classic forms [3]. In effect, classic CdLS is more commonly seen from variants of proteins that regulate cohesin functioning (NIPBL, HDAC8), whereas non-classic or milder CdLS phenotypes are often attributed to variants in genes that encode structural units of the cohesin complex (SMC1A, RAD21, SMC3) or cohesin-associated proteins (ANKRD11, BRD4, and others).

Investigations over the years have consistently reported variability in the severity of the clinical phenotype across gene variants causative of CdLS. Individuals with non-classic CdLS tend to present with less severe limb or structural abnormalities [2, 8, 10, 16, 19], craniofacial profile [2, 8, 10, 16], growth delays (e.g., weight, head circumference, [8, 10, 16]), and cognitive challenges [7, 9, 10]. However, to our knowledge, to date, the genotype–phenotype association in neurobehavioral and developmental profiles of CdLS remains a gap in literature. Providing more precise phenotype characterization across genotype enables more focused clinical care and can shed light onto the complex psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the range of CdLS presentations.

Accordingly, this study examines the behavior, developmental and intervention histories of 38 individuals with CdLS (26 with a pathogenic variant in NIPBL, 12 with an SMC1A variant). Data utilized in this study stem from developmental inventories completed by caregivers who participated in the Coordination of Rare Diseases (CoRDS) registry at Sanford. Consistent with the body of literature that highlights more severe clinical symptoms among those with NIPBL than those with other CdLS-causative variants [12, 14], we hypothesized that those with a pathogenic variant in NIPBL will present with more severe developmental delays, enroll in more intervention services (speech, occupational, and/or physical therapies) and demonstrate more challenging behaviors than those with an SMC1A variant.

Methods

Clinical sample and procedures

Data of 38 individuals with CdLS from the CoRDS registry were included in this study, 26 with a pathogenic variant in NIPBL and 12 with one in SMC1A. Information regarding the variant type was not collected by the registry. Participant characteristics and behavioral/attention composite scores are outlined in Table 1. Although both groups yielded similar range in age of participants, the NIPBL group was generally older (NIPBL: Mean age = 20.46 years, SD = 11.21, range 5–36; SMC1A: Mean age = 11.08 years, SD = 9.04, range = 5–38; F = 6.42, p = 0.016, η2 = 0.15). Racial composition between the two groups differed. The SMC1A group had more diverse representation whereas participants with NIPBL variant are all White. Both groups were comparable in other sociodemographic information including sex and insurance coverage, which was considered a proxy of socioeconomic status. All participants reside in the United States.

Table 1 Participant characteristics, intervention history, and behavioral functioning

Patient-reported data from CoRDS (https://research.sanfordhealth.org/rare-disease-registry) were accessed as part of this study. Caregivers and parents of individuals with a diagnosis of CdLS age 5 years and older completed a battery of surveys to obtain natural history. Demographic, medical, neurologic, developmental and behavioral information were inquired across the inventories. Caregivers were asked to indicate the timeframe when their child achieved a milestone. The response options available were to check “< 12 months” or “other”, which subsequently prompted them to list the age (years) during which the child met the milestone. Participating caregivers also completed four rating items indexing attention and another four focused on repetitive behaviors, all of which were on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = Never a problem, 1 = Not a problem today but in the past, 2 = Currently a problem). The surveys that caregivers completed were designed by both clinical and scientific advisors from the Cornelia de Lange Foundation (CdLS) and CoRDS, a non-profit organization that works with patient advocacy groups and researchers to collect clinical information related to rare diseases in a standardized fashion.

Exclusion criteria primarily consisted of individuals who did not report genetic variant resulting in the CdLS diagnosis. This study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Johns Hopkins Medicine.

Data strategy

The first author analyzed the anonymized patient data with SPSS 26.0. The number of patients who reported weekly to monthly participation in speech/language, occupational, and physical therapies was tabulated and summed into Total Intervention Use (e.g., 0 refers to no inventions, 3 refers to participation in all three therapies). Likewise, the proportion of patients who met a language or motor developmental milestone by the first 5 years of life was tallied. Ratings for the 8 items indexing attention and rigid behaviors were aggregated into two composite scores (Attention Total, Repetitive Behaviors Total). History of self-injurious behaviors and anxiety were dichotomously coded (yes/no significant history).

Non-parametric (Mann Whitney U test) was used to examine group differences in age at meeting developmental milestones, attention/behavior composites as well as cumulative intervention use given tests of normality suggested data were not normally distributed. Linear regression models were used to examine the association between genotype and language/motor developmental milestones after controlling for age at survey completion. Fishers exact test was applied to determine group differences in sociodemographic variables, the proportion of patients who never mastered a language/motor skill, and the proportion of patients endorsing specific attention, hyperactive and repetitive behaviors.

Results

Developmental milestones and intervention services

Of the whole sample, 17 caregivers of the NIPBL variant group and 11 of the SMC1A variant group completed a developmental survey regarding the timeframe their child met major motor and language milestones. Figures 1A, B and 2A, B illustrate the proportion of the clinical sample that met language and motor milestones over the first 5 years of life. No group differences were observed in the attainment of early language and motor milestones. Across the two clinical groups, the proportion of participants who did not meet later language milestones (Single Word Use, Two Word Sentence Use) were similar, with about 70% of both groups not mastering two-word sentences by the survey completion (i.e., at least 5 years of age) (Fig. 3A, B). About 30% of the SMC1A and 50% of the NIPBL variant groups were not walking unassisted by the time of survey completion, albeit the group effect did not reach significance. Notably, as outlined in Table 2, when examining the subset of participants who reported meeting the milestone, those with NIBPL variants were less delayed than those with SMC1A variant during early communication development (Social Smile, Utterance, Single Word Use). However, these patients were on average more delayed and more variable in their attainment of more complex language (Two Word Sentence Use). Both groups were similarly delayed in meeting motor milestones, albeit those with NIPBL variants similarly showed more variability in the age of achieving milestone for more advanced gross motor functions (Walking Unassisted). While both groups are significantly delayed relative to the developmental milestones released by the CDC [6], they show some similarities in language progression based on the published developmental data of a mixed group of individuals with CdLS [15]. However, later motor functions like Walking Unassisted yielded different developmental milestones among our groups relative to the sample in Kline et al. [15]. Among the participants with an NIPBL variant, Walking Unassisted was on average a year more delayed than the reported timeframe in Kline et al. [15] which may stem from different operationalization of milestones as highlighted in Discussion below.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A and B Proportion of the patients who met the language developmental milestones by 5 years of age

Fig. 2
figure 2

A and B Proportion of the patients who met the motor developmental milestones by 5 years of age

Fig. 3
figure 3

Proportion of patients with mutation in NIPBL or SMC1A who did not meet the language or motor milestone by 5 years of age

Table 2 Mean age of meeting milestone (years) among the patients who reported meeting the milestone by the time of study participation. Standard deviation and range are provided in parentheses and brackets

Participation in speech and occupational interventions were comparable across both groups, but those with SMC1A variants were more likely to engage in physical therapy (FET p = 0.05) and use of communication devices (FET p = 0.08) (Table 1).

Behavioral phenotype

Caregivers’ ratings implicate a history of autistic features more prominently in the NIPBL variant group, meaning individuals are more likely affected by severe behavioral difficulties currently or in the past. Indeed, those with NIPBL variants yielded more elevated Repetitive Behavior Total compared to the SMC1A variant group but not Attention Problem Total (Table 1). Table 3 outlines the proportion of participants across variant groups that endorsed behavioral challenges. Specifically, these individuals were more likely to endorse a history of withdrawn or socially isolating behaviors (NIPBL: 65%, SMC1A: 18%), temper tantrums (NIPBL: 70%, SMC1A: 27%), hard to reach or get information through (NIPBL: 65%, SMC1A: 44%), and stereotypies like spinning (NIPBL: 75%, SMC1A: 18%). Self-injurious behaviors were observed among more than half of those with an NIPBL variant but none of those with an SMC1A variant. Likewise, those with an NIPBL variant were more likely to have a history of anxiety.

Table 3 The percentage of respondents that endorsed problem behaviors

More respondents from the SMC1A group reported a history of hyperactivity (NIPBL: 32%, SMC1A: 70%). No other group differences in attention were observed although a large proportion of both samples endorsed a significant history of restlessness (NIPBL: 55%, SMC1A: 70%), challenges with paying attention to directions (NIPBL: 86%, SMC1A: 80%), and distractibility (NIPBL: 86%, SMC1A: 82%), underscoring the importance of treating attention and hyperactivity symptoms to improve quality of life.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study focused on examining genotype–phenotype relationships in developmental and behavioral functioning among those with CdLS. Importantly, unlike many investigations involving retrospective chart reviews—which often incorporate mixed methods, clinician judgment and measurement tools—this study involved prospective standardized data collection as part of patients’ caregiver participation in a rare disease registry (CoRDS). Main findings generally suggest similar developmental delays in early communicative and motor development across those with a variant in either NIPBL and SMC1A, albeit those with NIPBL variants showed greater variability in the achievement of more complex language and motor milestones. Interestingly, those with NIPBL variants appear to present with a more severe behavioral phenotype. In brief, individuals with CdLS would benefit from early neuropsychological or neurodevelopmental assessments to identify individual developmental patterns, inform early intervention services (e.g., speech/language, physical, behavioral health therapies), and assist in connecting families to appropriate assistive technology specialists as warranted.

In contrast to prior research suggesting more cognitive delay among those with an NIPBL variant [7, 9, 10, 12], our findings suggest similar delays in early motor and communicative/language development. Discrepancies in findings can stem from low sample sizes and focused clinical samples (e.g., our inclusion of only individuals with gene variants and not deletions). The lack of variant status leaves challenges in our data interpretation, as we are unable to determine whether the NIPBL or SMC1A groups are largely comprised of a specific variant type. Prospective studies should consider the variant type as recent studies in CdLS show individuals with truncating variants present with more autism-related features and communication challenges than those with non-truncating (e.g., missense) variants [1]. Moreover, our study design was constrained by deidentified survey data provided through CoRDS, such that it was not possible to inquire caregivers to clarify the specific months when developmental milestones were met. Unlike prior studies on CdLS and development [15] which included more detailed review of early developmental achievements, inventories used here afford approximations of early language and motor gains but have limited sensitivity in discerning the extent of delays. For example, two children may achieve single word use at 6 months versus 11 months of age, and parents similarly check the response option of “< 12 months” when one is meeting the milestone early and the other on time. Likewise, caregivers of two children who walked at 18 months and 24 months of age may indicate they met the milestone at 2 years of age suggesting both are similarly delayed when in fact the former child achieved it broadly within normal limits. While our measures have their flaws, it is important to underscore that our study utilized a standardized approach to obtain neurodevelopmental information of patients, whereas many prior genotype–phenotype studies provide vague information as to how cognitive functioning was assessed or utilize heterogeneous measures (i.e., retrospective chart reviews with different assessments across patients). In brief, future research should consider utilizing more nuanced questions to index early developmental milestones achievements (e.g., age in months, qualitative information on developmental history), and integration of standardized developmental screening measures such as the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales or the Developmental Profile 4th Edition that has normative comparison data and yields age equivalents to assess the extent of delay. Indeed, recent investigations using standardized cognitive measures in older school-age children with CdLS have revealed more severe autistic features and challenges across communication and motor skills among those with NIPBL truncating variants than in those with missense variants [1]. Adopting a similar approach using standardized developmental assessments will be helpful to determine if such patterns are seen in early childhood or if these trends evolve over time.

It is important to note that those with NIPBL variants yielded greater variability in achieving more advanced language and motor milestones, if at all. This could be due to molecular factors, such as whether the variant was a missense vs. loss of function variant, or the location along the gene. However, divergent patterns between our findings with prior investigations that highlighted greater cognitive deficit in those with NIPBL variants [7, 9, 10, 12], may also reflect more limited developmental gains over time in these individuals compared to those with an SMC1A variant. In addition, the operationalization of cognitive impairment across investigations varied, as some described the extent of intellectual disability, including adaptive functioning and life skills, whereas others reported “cognitive delays” in their sample. It is also unclear if cognition was indexed by intellectual functioning, nonverbal reasoning skills, or receptive language or language comprehension particularly if a large proportion of those affected by CdLS may not master phrase or sentence speech.

To our knowledge, our study first documents differences in intervention history across genotype linked to CdLS. Those with SMC1A variants were more likely to use communicative devices and engage in physical therapy than those with NIPBL variants despite similar delays in later milestones (e.g., sentence use, walking). It is possible that more substantial early childhood delays in speech/language functions among those with SMC1A variants drove families to initiate developmental surveillance and treatments more promptly, thus, resulting in more timely integration of communication technology.

Additionally, more severe behavioral phenotypes, including autistic features, anxiety, and self-injurious behaviors, were observed in those with NIPBL variants, supporting a genotype-focused approach in treatment planning among those with CdLS. From a clinical standpoint, healthcare and school providers working with individuals with CdLS may consider integration of behavior intervention services in outpatient and school settings. Structured behavioral treatment approaches such as applied behavior analyses (ABA) may be beneficial for these children to support their social communication and behavior regulation skills. Regarding future research directions, more extensive genotype–phenotype investigations utilizing a comprehensive battery of multiple informant inventories in addition to clinical structured interviews/observations will be essential to delineate the shared versus unique behavioral traits among those with different CdLS-causative gene variants. Longitudinal investigations with larger samples of patients with CdLS-causing gene variants (e.g., inclusion of those with pathogenic variants in HDAC8, SMC3, etc.), comparison groups of idiopathic autism spectrum disorder and/or intellectual disability, and other clinical groups with overlapping CdLS-like features, such as pathogenic variants in EP300, AFF4, NAA10, and TAF6, are necessary to disentangle the different disease pathways that underline the CdLS neurobehavioral phenotype. In addition to behavioral measures, mixed methods including biological metrics such as functioning magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), cortisol reactivity, and electroencephalogram (EEG) are vital to understand the effect of genotype anomalies on brain-behavior development. Ultimately, literature resulting from these efforts will be central to consider in later construction of clinical trials.

Study limitations and future directions

Despite the novelty of our findings, several study limitations should be considered. Given our limited participant pool, our study had low statistical power and poor sensitivity to detect more nuanced developmental differences between variant groups. As noted above, larger samples and appropriate comparison groups are needed to clarify genotype–phenotype relationships. More comprehensive behavioral and developmental assessment measures including those with normative comparison samples, and neurobiological metrics should be applied in subsequent research in CdLS. A developmental approach should be applied in examining progress across language and motor functions, and the extent these affect higher order cognition such as abstract reasoning and problem-solving. Given our findings, detailed review of intervention history in relation to developmental surveillance should be considered to determine whether observed greater variability in later language and motor skills in those with NIPBL variants may stem from differences in the initiation of or intensity of treatment services. Applying a developmental framework in CdLS research would be essential to understand the effect the pathogenic variant in CdLS-causative genes has on the maturation processes of focal neural networks. So, too, comparing the specific types of variants in the various genes, juxtaposed on both development and behavior, may be of benefit.

In brief, results from this study show significant but comparable developmental delays in early childhood between those with NIPBL vs SMC1A variants; however, those with NIPBL variants present with more severe behavioral challenges. Findings support consideration of more genotype-focused program of treatments given differences in phenotypic severity.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from CoRDS Registry but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. Data are however available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of Sanford.

References

  1. Ajmone PF, Allegri B, Cereda A, Michelini G, Dall’Ara F, Mariani M, Rigamonti C, Selicorni A, Vizziello P, Costantino MAJ. Neuropsychiatric functioning in CdLS: A detailed phenotype and genotype correlation. Autism Dev Disord. 2022;52:4763–4773. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10803-021-05343-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Allanson JE, Hennekam RC, Ireland M. De Lange syndrome: subjective and objective comparison of the classical and mild phenotypes. J Med Genet. 1997;34(8):645–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Ansari M, Poke G, Ferry Q, Williamson K, Aldridge R, Meynert AM, Bengani H, Chan CY, Kayserili H, Avci Ş, Hennekam RC, FitzPatrick DR. Genetic heterogeneity in Cornelia de Lange syndrome (CdLS) and CdLS-like phenotypes with observed and predicted levels of mosaicism. J Med Genet. 2014;51(10):659–68.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Basile E, Villa L, Selicorni A, Molteni M. The behavioural phenotype of Cornelia de Lange syndrome: a study of 56 individuals. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51(9):671–81.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Baranano KW, Kimball A, Fong SL, Egense AS, Hudon C, Kline AD. Further characterization of SMC1A loss of function epilepsy distinct from Cornelia de Lange syndrome. J Child Neurol. 2022;37:390–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Developmental surveillance resources for healthcare providers. 2022. www.cdc.gov/ActEarly/healthcare.

  7. Deardorff MA, Bando M, Nakato R, Watrin E, Itoh T, Minamino M, Saitoh K, Komata M, Katou Y, Clark D, Cole KE, Shirahige K. HDAC8 mutations in Cornelia de Lange syndrome affect the cohesin acetylation cycle. Nature. 2012;489(7415):313–7.

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Deardorff MA, Kaur M, Yaeger D, Rampuria A, Korolev S, Pie J, Gil-Rodríguez C, Arnedo M, Loeys B, Kline AD, Wilson M, Krantz ID. Mutations in cohesin complex members SMC3 and SMC1A cause a mild variant of Cornelia de Lange syndrome with predominant mental retardation. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;80(3):485–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Deardorff MA, Wilde JJ, Albrecht M, Dickinson E, Tennstedt S, Braunholz D, Mönnich M, Yan Y, Xu W, Gil-Rodríguez MC, Clark D, Kaiser FJ. RAD21 mutations cause a human cohesinopathy. Am J Hum Genet. 2012;90(6):1014–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Gil-Rodríguez MC, Deardorff MA, Ansari M, Tan CA, Parenti I, Baquero-Montoya C, Ousager LB, Puisac B, Hernández-Marcos M, Teresa-Rodrigo ME, Marcos-Alcalde I, Pié J. De Novo heterozygous mutations in SMC3 cause a range of Cornelia de Lange syndrome-overlapping phenotypes. Hum Mutat. 2015;36(4):454–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Huisman S, Mulder PA, Redeker E, Bader I, Bisgaard AM, Brooks A, Cereda A, Cinca C, Clark D, Cormier-Daire V, Deardorff MA, Hennekam RC. Phenotypes and genotypes in individuals with SMC1A variants. Am J Med Genet A. 2017;173(8):2108–25.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Kaur M, Blair J, Devkota B, Fortunato S, Clark D, Lawrence A, Kim J, Do W, Semeo B, Katz O, Mehta D, Krantz ID. Genomic analyses in Cornelia de Lange Syndrome and related diagnoses: novel candidate genes, genotype–phenotype correlations and common mechanisms. Am J Med Genet Part A. 2023;191:2113–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kline AD, Krantz ID, Sommer A, Kliewer M, Jackson LG, FitzPatrick DR et al. Cornelia de Lange syndrome: clinical review, diagnostic and scoring systems, and anticipatory guidance. Am J Med Genet Part A. 2007;143(12):1287–1296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Kline AD, Moss JF, Selicorni A, Bisgaard AM, Deardorff MA, Gillett PM, Ishman SL, Kerr LM, Levin AV, Mulder PA, Ramos FJ, Hennekam RC. Diagnosis and management of Cornelia de Lange syndrome: first international consensus statement. Nat Rev Genet. 2018;19(10):649–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Kline AD, Stanley C, Belevich J, Brodsky K, Barr M, Jackson LG. Developmental data on individuals with the Brachmann–de Lange syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1993;47:1053–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Mannini L, Cucco F, Quarantotti V, Krantz ID, Musio A. Mutation spectrum and genotype–phenotype correlation in Cornelia de Lange syndrome. Hum Mutat. 2013;34(12):1589–96.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Moss J, Howlin P, Magiati I, Oliver C. Characteristics of autism spectrum disorder in Cornelia de Lange syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):883–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Olley G, Ansari M, Bengani H, Grimes GR, Rhodes J, von Kriegsheim A, Blatnik A, Stewart FJ, Wakeling E, Carroll N, Ross A, FitzPatrick DR. BRD4 interacts with NIPBL and BRD4 is mutated in a Cornelia de Lange-like syndrome. Nat Genet. 2018;50(3):329–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Rohatgi S, Clark D, Kline AD, Jackson LG, Pie J, Siu V, Ramos FJ, Krantz ID, Deardorff MA. Facial diagnosis of mild and variant CdLS: insights from a dysmorphologist survey. Am J Med Genet A. 2010;152(7):1641–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Srivastava S, Landy-Schmitt C, Clark B, Kline AD, Specht M, Grados MA. Autism traits in children and adolescents with Cornelia de Lange syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2014;164(6):1400–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, Whitaker TM, Shaw JS, Squires JK, Pajek JA, Wolf RB, Slaughter KS, Broughton AS, Gerndt KL, Lipkin PH. Evidence-informed milestones for developmental surveillance tools. Pediatrics. 2022;149(3):e2021052138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Cornelia de Lange Syndrome Foundation for initiating the project, the patients and their families who participated in this study, as well as acknowledging the support of Coordination of Rare Diseases (CoRDS) registry at Sanford Research.

Funding

R.N. receives funding from the Wiedemann-Steiner Syndrome Foundation and KAT6 Foundation, and support from the Kennedy Krieger/Johns Hopkins IDDRC NIH P50HD103538 and ICTR (UL1 TR003098) as well as NIH R25 NS117356. 

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

J.O’C. and A.D.K. helped draft the original survey questions. R.N. conceived the study and designed the project. R.N. wrote the first draft of the manuscript with feedback from A.D.K. D.S. helped coordinate the collection of surveys from the families. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rowena Ng.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The institutional review board at Johns Hopkins Medicine approved study procedures.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ng, R., O’Connor, J., Summa, D. et al. Neurobehavioral and developmental profiles: genotype–phenotype correlations in individuals with Cornelia de Lange syndrome. Orphanet J Rare Dis 19, 111 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13023-024-03104-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13023-024-03104-1

Keywords